AGE AND THE PERCEPTION OF A MODULATING TRAFFIC SIGNAL LIGHT IN A FIELD LOCATION

Similar documents
Be sure to check the camera is properly functioning, is properly positioned and securely mounted, every time you operate your vehicle.

STX Stairs lighting controller.

D.A. Schreuder Voorburg, ]981 Institute for Road Safety Research SWOV, The Netherlands

September 28, 2018 CITY OF BERKELEY JOHN MUIR SCHOOL CROSSING IMPROVEMENTS SPECIFICATION NO C ADDENDUM NO. 2

RECOMMENDED AMENDMENTS TO OFF-PREMISE SIGN REGULATIONS

STANDARDISATION OF LIGHT SIGNALS FOR ROAD TRAFFIC CONTROL

THE USE OF LED LAMPS FOR TURN AND STOP SIGNAL PRESENTATIONS

Technology Control Technology

What is the lowest contrast spatial frequency you can see? High. x x x x. Contrast Sensitivity. x x x. x x. Low. Spatial Frequency (c/deg)

EVALUATION OF PERFORMANCE OF SOLAR POWERED FLASHING BEACONS AT ROOM TEMPERATURE CONDITIONS

Atlas Drop In Decoder

Safety Information. Camera System. If you back up while looking only at the monitor, you may cause damage or injury. Always back up slowly.

21-Pin Connector. Dimensions: 1.28 x.69 x.22 or 32.5mm x 17.5mm x 5.6mm

Dynamic Animation Cube Group 1 Joseph Clark Michael Alberts Isaiah Walker Arnold Li

HO-Scale Kato DCC Sound Conversion Kit

MEDICAL UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH CAROLINA DEPARTMENT OF PUBLIC SAFETY

Main Features of this Decoder

Smart Traffic Signs. I can communicate! By: Salil S. Gokhale Preston DeFrancis

Public Opinion and Understanding of Advance Warning Arrow Displays Used in Short-Term, Mobile, and Moving Work Zones

Technical Note. Flicker

HO-Scale Athearn DCC Sound Conversion Kit

Traffic Control Device Evaluation Program: FY 2016

Optical Signal Devices. Free-standing Beacons

ALO 030 MKII. 30 Watt DMX LED scanner. User manual

The National Traffic Signal Report Card: Highlights

INTERIM ADVICE NOTE 109/08. Advice Regarding the Motorway Signal Mark 4 (MS4)

City of Tacoma Community and Economic Development Department

A Case Study: Complex Accident Reconstruction from Video Footage

Brief History of the MUTCD

Processes for the Intersection

ECE 4600 PROJECT. The 13 State Traffic Controller. Group #3 Anthony Ostrowski Jeffrey Ostrowski Derek Zoldos

Digital High Resolution Display Technology. A New Way of Seeing Things.

A.D. Engineering International Pty Ltd - Product Range

SPEED LIMIT YOUR SPEED SPEED LIMIT YOUR SPEED SPEED LIMIT YOUR SPEED SCHOOL YOUR SPEED. Information Display Company WHEN CHILDREN ARE PRESENT

Aerial Cable Installation Best Practices

MICROMASTER Encoder Module

Attachment N - General Specifications for LED Luminaires. General Specifications for Solid State Lighting LED Roadway Luminaires

TIL311 HEXADECIMAL DISPLAY WITH LOGIC

Azatrax Model Railroad Track Signal Control - Single Track

NEW APPROACHES IN TRAFFIC SURVEILLANCE USING VIDEO DETECTION

Optical Flicker Explained. A Plain English Version of Flicker Considerations in Design

TP /2017-E POWER LED 300. Luminous. Efficient. Durable.

DIGITAL SIGN SURVEY SURVEY REQUESTED BY CYLCE JOHNSON ON 2/26/07 - QUESTION: NAHBA SURVEY ON SIGN INTENSITY (BRIGHTNESS)

LMT X1 - a 12.1" VHB

Operating Manual (Edition 04/2004) sinamics. Line Reactors SINAMICS G130

Guidelines for Specification of LED Lighting Products 2010

Do not install and/or operate this safety product unless you have read and understand the safety information contained in this manual.

Machine Vision System for Color Sorting Wood Edge-Glued Panel Parts

Instruction Manual. 2.4G Digital Wireless Four Channel Transmitter System RVS-554W. Reverse With Confidence 1

icolor COVE LT SPECIFICATIONS output range 110 x 50 Two-piece vented plastic C-UL US Listed, CE certified COMMUNICATION SPECIFICATIONS

nroads Fleet Full Size Lightbar Mid-Size Lightbar Mini Lightbar Beacons Accessories

iphone Accelerometer Results From 45 Emergency Braking Tests on Snow-Covered Roads

Operating Instructions DCI-50-CT Cable Identifier

Data Sheet. Electronic displays

A Directional Indicator with a Symbol Based Interface for improvised Safety-Enhanced Directional Indicators using Matrix LED

ARTICLE 8 - SIGN REGULATIONS

Before the Federal Communications Commission Washington, D.C

User Manual LivingColors Bloom

MATERIAL SPECIFICATION FOR SIGNAL HEADS

FINAL CONTRACT REPORT IMPROVEMENT OF CONSPICUITY OF TRAILBLAZING SIGNS: PHASE III-EVALUATION OF FLUORESCENT COLORS VICKI L. NEALE, RICHARD L.

Tech Paper. HMI Display Readability During Sinusoidal Vibration

DLP Discovery Reliability Application Note

Q1. Do LED lights burn out?

Fixed Signals - Rules 1 to 23

POINTS POSITION INDICATOR PPI4

Introduction. Characteristics (Note 1, 2) Parameters Typical Value Units Conditions

CRITICAL FREQUENCY OF FLICKER AS A FUNCTION OF INTENSITY OF ILLUMINATION FOR THE EYE OF THE BEE

Module No GENERAL GUIDE TIMEKEEPING MODE BACKLIGHT

Guidance on Lineside Signals, Indicators and Layout of Signals. Rail Industry Guidance Note for GK/RT0045

Trial decision. Conclusion The trial of the case was groundless. The costs in connection with the trial shall be borne by the demandant.

Transportation Engineering -II Dr. Rajat Rastogi Department of Civil Engineering Indian Institute of Technology - Roorkee

Application Note [AN-007] LCD Backlighting Technologies and Configurations

CROCODILE AUSTRIA VIDEOSYSTEM

120W Beam Light AL-BL-120 FEATURES. Lamp: PHILIPS 2R 120W. Channel mode: 20CH. Level scanning: 540 (16bit precision scan) electronic error correction

Controlling LEDs. why various control types are needed the existing methodologies used to control LEDs. Technical white paper January 2011

ULTRA-TRAC APL INSTRUCTION MANUAL. Read and understand instructions before use. Patented. 851 Transport Drive Valparaiso, IN

SYNC-WIRED AND ELECTRONIC SECONDARY CLOCK MOVEMENT COURSE GUIDELINES

51109 Köln St. Asaph, Denbigshire LL17OLJ

I 2. Government Accession No. 3. Recipient's Catalog No. I

Optical Engine Reference Design for DLP3010 Digital Micromirror Device

ECE Real Time Embedded Systems Final Project. Speeding Detecting System

VISUAL MILL LAB. SECTION 1: Complete the following tests and fill out the appropriate sections on your Visual Mill Color Deficit Worksheet.

Touch Panel RGB LED Controller Part No. touch-panel-rgb

Operating Instructions DCI-50-CT Cable Identifier

SPECIAL SPECIFICATION 8311 LED Countdown Pedestrian Signal Module

GORDON WATSON and ASSOCIATES

PoE: Adding Power to (IoT)

900-Lumen Portable LED Projector Part #: User manual

a lesson about embedding quotations

mirasol Display Value Proposition White Paper

LEGIBILITY DISTANCES OF 9 TO 18 INCH CHARACTERS FOR LIGHT-EMITTING DIODE (LED) DYNAMIC MESSAGE SIGNS ON ARTERIAL ROADWAYS

Under Vehicle. The most capable UVSS (Under Vehicle Surveillance System) available for detecting contraband, drugs, explosives, and other objects

This guide gives details of the effects available on the FX selection DMX channels 15 and 17 in the MAC Aura.

An Empirical Analysis of Macroscopic Fundamental Diagrams for Sendai Road Networks

LED driver architectures determine SSL Flicker,

FACTORY AUTOMATION AS-INTERFACE MAINTENANCE AND TROUBLESHOOTING GUIDE

User Manual MODEL: KKF1500-PCAP. True FLAT P-CAP LCD Monitor. Installation Guide. 15 True FLAT P-CAP Touch LCD Monitor

Introduction. Characteristics (Note 1, 2) Parameters Typical Value Units Conditions

Surgical Solutions. Brilliance. Focus. Comfort. HarmonyAIR M-Series Surgical Lighting System. One Integrated Approach to Healthcare

If you are using a CRT tester/restorer other than the CR7000 you are likely incorrectly testing today s CRTs and risk damaging every one you restore.

Transcription:

PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS SOCIETY 35th ANNUAL MEETING-1991 AGE AND THE PERCEPTION OF A MODULATING TRAFFIC SIGNAL LIGHT IN A FIELD LOCATION Lawrence T. Guzy and Nancy Pena-Reynolds State University of New York at Oneonta Richard D. Brugger Industrial Control Associates, Erie, PA. Herschel W. Leibowitz The Pennsylvania State University ABSTRACT Young, middle, and older drivers were motored towards a traffic signal face and were required to report whether the illuminated signal lens was modulating. No significant differences were found among the three age groups in the distance at which modulation was first reported nor in the change of the modulation pattern as they approached the signal face. The modulating of the illuminated green lens was perceived significantly further away than was the red lens. These distances increased during trials associated with inclement weather. Seventy-four percent of the observers indicated that the modulation attracted their attention. Of these, 64% reported a preference for the use of the modulating light in the traffic signals as compared with the standard light source. Implications for improving safety at intersections for the older driver are discussed. INTRODUCTION At intersections controlled by a traffic signal light the ability to detect the presence of a signal face and respond appropriately is a critical activity in a vehicular oriented environment. However, on occasion, drivers and pedestrians may respond inappropriately. These situations may be the result of risk taking, inattention, distraction, or looking but failing 'to see (Hills, 1980), and these are compounded by competing envi ronmental arrays that reduce the conspicuity of the illuminated signal lens. The use of flashing lights has been successfully used to improve the conspicuity of two traffic control devices. Strybel & Nassi (1986) improved the recognition of a lane reversing signal under conditions of sun glare by placing a rotating beacon above the signal; and a strobe light strip has been attached to the face of the rea lens and pulses when the red signal illuminated (MUTCD, 1988). Although effective, a potential problem exists. These al erti ng devices are tangenti al to the signals and their failure could result in misidentification of the signal lights from observers who are familiar with their operation. Recently, Brugger (1989) designed a dual-filament traffic signal light bulb where the warning was inherent in the signal's normal lighting pattern. By alternately energizing each filament, a modulation pattern was generated which observers describe as a "flickering or shimmering" light. Preliminary research with observers who were not informed of the shimmering light responded appropriately to the red and green signal lights. A rate of four hertz was identified as this rate produced the maximum distance for viewing modulation and was not confused with a "blinking" traffic signal light. The observers independently reported that the modulation pattern was qualitatively different from what they have previously experienced. Guzy, Suib, Leibowitz, and Brugger (1990) examined the effects of this light source in a traffic signal face under conditions of simulated 1130

PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS SOCIETY 35th ANNUAL MEETING 1991 sun backlighting. No errors involving recognition and driving were found with the modulating light source in the red or green lenses. Several errors were found with the standard traffic signal bulb. Here, the drivers incorrectly identified the illuminated lens and either stopped for a green light or passed a red light. As compared with younger drivers, older drivers may benefit more from the modulating light source placed in the signal head. Whereas accidents involving younger drivers are attributed to such factors as speeding and driving while intoxicated, the older driver fails to heed signs, yield the right of way, or turn properly (Planek, 1973; Huston & Janke, 1986). The modulating light bulb may improve signal recognition and reduce such driving errors. The present research was designed to examine whether age related visual changes affect the recognition, distance, and quality of the modulation effect. As older drivers prefer daylight hours for driving, the research was conducted under photopic conditions. Method Subjects. Twenty-eight licensed men and women drivers were paid $5.00 for their participation. Subjects formed three age groups: a) youngages 20 to 32, n = 9, mean age of 22 years, b) mid-ages 38 to 54, n = 9, mean age of 46 years, and c) older-ages 59 to 80, n = 10, mean age of 66 years. Nine men were found to be red-green color deficient. Each age group had at least two red-green color deficient observers. Visual Screening. Visual acuity was determined with a Bausch and Lomb Orthorater using only the far visual acuity plates. Color deficiencies were identified with the Dvorine Pseudo-Isochromatic Plates color vision test. Procedure. Observers sat on the passenger side of a full-sized vehicle driven at a speed of approximately 26 km. toward a three light traffic signal face. The bottom of the signal face was 4.92 m. from the road surface. Green and red lenses, 30.5 cm. dia., of the traffic light were illuminated by either a 150 watt Hytron bulb or a dual filament bulb modulating at a rate of 4 hz. Bulbs were matched for brightness. Subjects began viewing the traffic light from a distance of 262 m. and were required to identify the color and amount of flicker, if any, using a five point scale, and any other unusual aspects of the traffic light. Distances from the light were recorded with a NuMetrics Roadstar Model 20 computer connected to the vehicle's transmission. Eight different lighting conditions were administered twice for a total of sixteen trials. Each colored lens was placed in the top position of the traffic signal face for eight trials and in the bottom for the remaining 8 trials. The modulating and standard bulbs appeared equally often in each of these positions. Trials were block randomized. A post-test interview focused on observers impressions of the modulating light bulb. RESULTS The modulating effect of the dualfilament bulb produced reports of both quantitative and qualitative changes as subjects approached the traffic signal face. No significant difference were found among the three age groups. Within each age group large intersubject variabi l ity was found. With the Orthorater, visual performance on the far distance plates decrease with increasing age. However, the perception of the modulation pattern showed no relationship with Orthorater performance. The modulating pattern in the green and red lenses produced differential distance effects. The green lens was perceived as showing "some modu l at i on s i gn i f i cant l y further away (171 m., sd = 38.8) than was the red lens (130.8 m., sd = 53.5), E(1,54)=16.2, p<.001). A change in the reported modulation 1131

PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS SOCIETY 35th A ANNUAL MEETING 1991 pattern to "more" followed a similar pattern, F(1,51)=8.6, p<.05. During inclement weather, i.e., rain, fog, and overcast skies, the modulation pattern was seen further away than under sunny conditions. A significant inverse relationship was found between the report of "some" modulation and weather for both the green and red lenses, r=-.47, df=26, p(.01 and r =-.35, df=26, p<.05, respectively. A post-test interview showed that 64% of the subjects preferred the modulating light over the standard light. Two observers reported that the modulating light "attracted their r attention" and found it "annoying". Of the nine red/green color deficient participants, none incorrectly identified the color of the illuminated lens regardless of the lens' position in the traffic signal face. All of these observers reported a preference for the modulating red lens while the normal color visioned subjects were mixed with respect to their preference. DISCUSSION The modulation effect showed no differences among the three age groups in either the range of modulation or in the qualitative changes associated with the modulation pattern. The large variabilities found within each age group may be attributed to the difficulty in conveying precise terminology to characterize the modulating light pattern and any subsequent changes as viewing distance decreased. When asked to supply their own descriptors to define the effect, observers showed little consistency. The most commonly elicited terms included " s h i mme r i ng, " f l i c ke r i ng, and "modulating". Changes in the weather from photopic to mesopic conditions increased the range of influence of the modulating light. Regardless of the weather conditions, the overall range of the modulating light was limited. However, perception of the modulation would allow amply time for drivers to initiate an approriate response under both normal and degraded vi ew i ng condi ti ions. In the present study, with uncompromised viewing conditions, 64% of the subjects indicated a preference for the modulating light in the traffic signal face. In a previous study which used a degraded viewing condition involving simulated sun backlighting of the traffic signal lenses, twelve of fourteen subjects who reported seeing the modulation were unanimous in their preference for the modulation. Of the two subjects who could not recall seeing the modulation, one committed several recognition and driving errors with the standard light source, but not with the modulating light (Guzy, et al., 1990). The difference in preferences may be attributable to a difference in procedures. In the earlier study, subjects were required to drive a three-wheeled vehicle along a prescribed route, identify signs placed along a simulated roadway, report the color of the illuminated lens, and at a simulated intersection respond accordingly, i.e., drive through or stop. The procedure in this study required the subject to identify the presence of modulation and to report any changes. Several of the passengers indicated that they had fixated on the illuminated lens for the entire duration of the trial (approx. 35 seconds) to the exclusion of all other visual i n f o r ma t i on. W i thou t these distractor tasks, the singular task of viewing the illuminated signal may account for the reduced preference and in two cases, annoyance. Given the difficulties associated with the older drivers, eg., heeding signs and responding to information in the periphery (Kosnik, Sekuler, & Kline, 1990), the use of the modulating light may possibly improve intersection safety by enhancing the detection of the traffic signal face and 1132

PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS SOClETY 35th ANNUAL MEETING 1991 recognition of the illuminated l ens. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors wish to thank Joyce A. Blake-Guzy for her invaluable assistance in data collection and to the Pennsylvania Transportation Institute for the use of their automotive test track for preliminary testing. The research was supported by a grant from the University Transportation Research Center, Federal Region 2. For specific information on modulating bulb characteristics contact Richard Brugger, P.E., Industrial Control Associates, P.O. Box 82,Erie, Pa. 16512. REFERENCES Brugger, R. D. (1989). Traffic Signal. United States Patent Office. Pat. No. 4,799,060. Patent Date 1/17/89. Kosnik, W. D., Sekuler, R., and Kline, D. W. (1990). Selfreported visual problems of older drivers. Human Factors, 32, 597 608. Huston, R. and Janke, M. (1986). Senior driver facts. Tech. Report CA L-DMV-RSS-86-82. Sacramento, CA. Department of Motor Vehicle. Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices ( 1988). Government Printing Office, Superintendent of Documents, Washington, D.C. 20402. Planek, T. W. (1973). The aging driver in today's traffic: A critical review. In Aging and high way safety: The elderly in a mobile society. North Carolina Symposium on Highway Safety. V.7. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina, Safety Research Center. Strybel, T. Z. and Nassi, R. (1986). Daylight conspicuity of reversible-lane signal systems. Human Factors. 28, 83-89. Guzy, L.T., Suib, S., Leibowitz, H.W., and Brugger, R. D. (1990). Improving the conspicuity of traffic signal lights. "Work i n Progress ". Human Factors Society Conference. Orl ando, FL. Hills, B. (1980). Vision, visibility, and perception in driving. Perception. 9. 183-216. 1133