Smooth Rhythms as Probes of Entrainment. Music Perception 10 (1993): ABSTRACT

Similar documents
OLCHS Rhythm Guide. Time and Meter. Time Signature. Measures and barlines

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Chapter 2: Beat, Meter and Rhythm: Simple Meters

Northeast High School AP Music Theory Summer Work Answer Sheet

RHYTHM. Simple Meters; The Beat and Its Division into Two Parts

Spatial-frequency masking with briefly pulsed patterns

Tempo and Beat Analysis

2 Autocorrelation verses Strobed Temporal Integration

MUSIC IN TIME. Simple Meters

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Perceiving temporal regularity in music

Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm and Timing 1

How to Obtain a Good Stereo Sound Stage in Cars

2005 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. The Influence of Pitch Interval on the Perception of Polyrhythms

LESSON 1 PITCH NOTATION AND INTERVALS

Tempo and Beat Tracking

Robert Alexandru Dobre, Cristian Negrescu

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England

Author Index. Absolu, Brandt 165. Montecchio, Nicola 187 Mukherjee, Bhaswati 285 Müllensiefen, Daniel 365. Bay, Mert 93

On time: the influence of tempo, structure and style on the timing of grace notes in skilled musical performance

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

OpenStax-CNX module: m Time Signature * Catherine Schmidt-Jones

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

AP Music Theory Summer Assignment

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

Tapping to Uneven Beats

AP MUSIC THEORY SUMMER ASSIGNMENT AP Music Theory Students and Parents,

The Lecture Contains: Frequency Response of the Human Visual System: Temporal Vision: Consequences of persistence of vision: Objectives_template

Modeling the Effect of Meter in Rhythmic Categorization: Preliminary Results

Brain.fm Theory & Process

The Mathematics of Music and the Statistical Implications of Exposure to Music on High. Achieving Teens. Kelsey Mongeau

Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords

AP Music Theory Assignment

Music Representations

Chapter 40: MIDI Tool

Controlling Musical Tempo from Dance Movement in Real-Time: A Possible Approach

MPATC-GE 2042: Psychology of Music. Citation and Reference Style Rhythm and Meter

CLASSIFICATION OF MUSICAL METRE WITH AUTOCORRELATION AND DISCRIMINANT FUNCTIONS

Teaching Total Percussion Through Fundamental Concepts

Psychoacoustics. lecturer:

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Differences in Metrical Structure Confound Tempo Judgments Justin London, August 2009

Chapter 2--Rhythm. Student: 1. The regular pulse in music is called the A. beat B. meter C. tempo D. rhythm E. measure

The Cocktail Party Effect. Binaural Masking. The Precedence Effect. Music 175: Time and Space

Fraction by Sinevibes audio slicing workstation

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

Rhythm and Transforms, Perception and Mathematics

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

Polyrhythms Lawrence Ward Cogs 401

ADVANCED STUDY GUIDE

Automatic music transcription

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

However, in studies of expressive timing, the aim is to investigate production rather than perception of timing, that is, independently of the listene

Combining Instrument and Performance Models for High-Quality Music Synthesis

Primo Theory. Level 7 Revised Edition. by Robert Centeno

Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

Classification of Dance Music by Periodicity Patterns

Music Complexity Descriptors. Matt Stabile June 6 th, 2008

DIGITAL COMMUNICATION

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

S I N E V I B E S FRACTION AUDIO SLICING WORKSTATION

A cross-cultural comparison study of the production of simple rhythmic patterns

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Supplemental Material for Gamma-band Synchronization in the Macaque Hippocampus and Memory Formation

Experiments on tone adjustments

Meter and Autocorrelation

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Violin timbre and the picket fence

The Generation of Metric Hierarchies using Inner Metric Analysis

A 5 Hz limit for the detection of temporal synchrony in vision

Musical Rhythm for Linguists: A Response to Justin London

Machine Learning Term Project Write-up Creating Models of Performers of Chopin Mazurkas

Beat - The underlying, evenly spaced pulse providing a framework for rhythm.

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

Rhythm: patterns of events in time. HST 725 Lecture 13 Music Perception & Cognition

DSP First Lab 04: Synthesis of Sinusoidal Signals - Music Synthesis

The Formation of Rhythmic Categories and Metric Priming

Investigation of Digital Signal Processing of High-speed DACs Signals for Settling Time Testing

FFT Laboratory Experiments for the HP Series Oscilloscopes and HP 54657A/54658A Measurement Storage Modules

Temporal control mechanism of repetitive tapping with simple rhythmic patterns

UNIT 1: QUALITIES OF SOUND. DURATION (RHYTHM)

Reference Manual. Using this Reference Manual...2. Edit Mode...2. Changing detailed operator settings...3

White Noise Suppression in the Time Domain Part II

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

INTERMEDIATE STUDY GUIDE

CTP 431 Music and Audio Computing. Basic Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology (GSCT) Juhan Nam

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

Temporal Coordination and Adaptation to Rate Change in Music Performance

Math and Music: The Science of Sound

Essentials Skills for Music 1 st Quarter

Standard 1: Singing, alone and with others, a varied repertoire of music

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

SWING, SWING ONCE MORE: RELATING TIMING AND TEMPO IN EXPERT JAZZ DRUMMING

Effects of articulation styles on perception of modulated tempos in violin excerpts

Effects of Tempo on the Timing of Simple Musical Rhythms

Rhythmic Dissonance: Introduction

Sight Singing & Ear Training I MUT 1241~ 1 credit

On the contextual appropriateness of performance rules

Transcription:

Smooth Rhythms as Probes of Entrainment Music Perception 10 (1993): 503-508 ABSTRACT If one hypothesizes rhythmic perception as a process employing oscillatory circuits in the brain that entrain to low-frequency periodicities in the neural firings evoked by an acoustic signal, then among the conceptually purest probes of those oscillatory circuits would be acoustic signals with only simple sinusoidal periodicities in the appropriate frequency range (perhaps from 0.3 Hz to 20 Hz). Such signals can be produced by the low-frequency amplitude modulation of an audible carrier wave by one or more sinusoids. The resulting rhythms are smooth in that their amplitude envelopes are smoothly varying with no obvious points of onset or offset. Because preliminary experiments with smooth rhythms have produced some unexpected results, and because smooth rhythms can be precisely controlled and varied (including, for example, the digital filtering of their Fourier components in the frequency domain), they are proposed as versatile stimuli for studies in rhythmic perception.

TEXT Experiments in rhythmic perception employ the stimuli thought to embody rhythm s fundamental elements. For instance, the short-long, 3/4-meter stimuli depicted in Figure 1, typical of innumerable studies, manifest clearly demarcated durations, precisely located points of attack, and easily derived temporal ratios. Yet if rhythmic perception is less a process of measurement and more one of the entrainment of oscillatory circuits in the brain to various periodicities in the neural firings evoked by music or speech (cf. Jones, 1986; Gjerdingen, 1989; Desain, 1992), then those kind of discontinuous stimuli would be among the worst imaginable: their instantaneous onsets and offsets would explosively jangle all such oscillatory circuits. (In this regard, note that researchers generally change idealized vertical onsets and offsets into sloping ramps. To do otherwise is to subject the listener to disturbing clicks. ). An array of oscillators with varying natural frequencies might better be probed by sinusoidal stimuli smooth rhythms. In the hope of advancing a discussion of this alternative paradigm, the following short report surveys results from some informal studies at Stony Brook. There small groups of professional and pre-professional musicians have been asked to comment on, or notate, the rhythms of various sinusoidal stimuli. Modulating an audible tone s amplitude with a single low-frequency sine wave produces, in most cases, the perception of an unending series of equal pulses or even beats. Musically more interesting rhythms result from modulating a tone s amplitude with the product of two low-frequency sine waves. For instance, if two such modulating waves have relative frequencies of f and 2f, then merely shifting the phase between them is sufficient to change the perceived rhythm from a pattern clearly evoking a 2/4 meter to one evoking a 3/4 meter.

Figure 1. A periodic amplitude envelope typical of stimuli used in tests of rhythmic perception. Such a stimulus, though atypical of music or speech, maximizes the researcher s ability to predetermine durations, attack points, and temporal ratios. A prototypical duple meter was perceived when the phase of the faster wave was shifted 45 behind that of the slower wave (see Figure 2 and Appendix). Most musicians transcribed the pattern in 2/4 (or 4/4) meter as a strong quarter (or half) note followed by two weak eighth (or quarter) notes, though about a fourth of the musicians heard it as a strong eighth (or quarter) note followed by three weak eighth (or quarter) notes. Musically, the pattern was strongly reminiscent of the di-di-dum, di-di-dum, di-di-dumrhythm of Rossini s William Tell Overture. If, however, the phase of the faster wave was shifted 90 behind that of the slower wave (see Figure 3 and Appendix), the result was a periodic amplitude

Figure 2. A periodic, smooth amplitude envelope perceived as an alternating series of a long and two shorts in a duple meter. envelope that musicians readily identified as a textbook case of an alternating series of shorts and longs in a 3/4 (or 3/8) meter. The musical effect of Figure 3 is that of a brisk waltz where quarter-note upbeats lead to half-note downbeats. At the perceived tempo (dotted half note = 80 bpm) there is little rhythmical sense of the undulation implied visually in Figure 3. Instead, the tones seem neatly articulated into upbeatdownbeat pairs: di-dum, di-dum, etc. Each tone s perceived moment of initiation or beat in Medieval musical terms, its ictus (pl., ictus) occurs near the halfway point on the ascending slope of each wave. As predicted in Gjerdingen (1988), the ictus thus seems to fall where the amplitude envelope changes from concave up to concave down, that is, where the second derivative of amplitude goes from positive to negative (see Figure 4: points A and B). Moreover, the resulting short-to-iong

Figure 3. A periodic, smooth amplitude envelope perceived as an alternating series of shorts and longs in a triple meter. ratio of 1.00:1.75 essentially matches Gabrielsson s (1988) average results of 1.00: 1.76 for a human performer. Shifting the phase of the faster wave 180 behind that of the slower wave produces the retrograde of Figure 3, in other words, the rhythm of Figure 3 played backwards. (To view the backward amplitude envelope, look at Figure 3 in a mirror.) Most music scholars would in principle deny the possibility that a regular alternation of shorts and longs in 3/4 meter could, when reversed, become other than another regular alternation of shorts and longs in 3/4 meter. Yet the backward version of this smooth rhythm strikes most musicians as a slightly jazzy example of 2/4 time (!) two quarter notes per measure grouped within the bar lines: daah-dip, daahdip, etc.

Figure 4. The smooth rhythm of Figure 3 (solid line) along with the second derivative (central second differences) of its time series of amplitudes (dashed line). In the forward direction, positive-to-negative zero-crossings of the second derivative occur at points A and B; in the reverse direction, at points M and N. These points are good predictors of perceived ictus, that is, moments of perceived onset, attack, or beat. If different oscillatory circuits in the brain respond individually to each lowfrequency periodicity in the amplitude envelope (thus performing a rough Fourier analysis of spectral magnitude), then the same set of oscillators would respond identically to the very different rhythms of Figures 2, 3, and 3-backwards. This undifferentiated response would be due to all three rhythms having been produced by the same three periodicities: the period of the perceived measure (frequency f), the period of half of the measure (2f), and the period of a third of the measure (3f, a sideband result of multiplying frequency f by 2f). For a theory of entrained

oscillators to explain how these very different rhythms would be perceived as different, either the oscillators would need to be responsive to phase relationships or they would need to process not the amplitude envelope as a whole but only extracted features of the envelope. For example, if the envelope of Figure 3 were reduced to just two spikes with the corresponding amplitudes and temporal locations of the predicted ictus (Figure 4, points A and B), then the oscillator representing triple subdivision of the perceived measure would respond considerably more strongly than the oscillator representing a duple subdivision. For Figure 3 played backwards, by contrast, a similar reduction to the predicted ictus (points M and N) would allow the oscillator representing duple subdivision to respond slightly more strongly than the oscillator representing triple subdivision (quintuple and septuple subdivisions would also be evident). These relationships conform to the perceived sense that the rhythm of Figure 3 is strongly triple when played forward, and less strongly duple when played backward. Smooth rhythms provide alternative stimuli for the reexamination of common rhythmic phenomena, and the notion of entrainment as a fundamental process of rhythmic perception provides a provocative conceptual basis for the reexamination of current explanations of those phenomena. The location of ictus in the above stimuli, for instance, is completely explained neither by threshold theories of the perceptual onsets of tones (Vos & Rasch [1981]) nor by theories of the perceptual centers of syllable onsets ( p-centers : Morton, Marcus, & Frankish [1976]; Howell [1988]). Likewise, without some clear theory of ictus such as is given by the secondderivative zero-crossing test, theories of rhythm based on duration will founder on smooth rhythms. I hope other scholars (and especially trained experimentalists) will find these smooth rhythms as intriguing as I do.

APPENDIX The stimuli were produced by modulating the amplitude of a 673 Hz sine-wave tone. The modulating signal for Figure 2 was defined as [(sin(t) + 1] [-cos(2t) + 1], where T = 2π(time in seconds)/0.743 and modulation was 100%. A unit value was added to each function to ensure that computations involved only positive numbers (a common requirement of neural-network modeling the results are different if negative value are introduced). The modulating signal for Figure 3 was defined as [(sin(t) + 1] [-sin(2t) + 1], and for Figure 3 played backwards as [(sin(t) + 1] [sin(2t) + 1]. The wave shown in Figure 3, if examined in detail, shows a tiny rise or hump in the interval between each perceived half note and the following quarter note. Played through loudspeakers, versions of Figure 3 both with and without the suppression of this hump were generally indistinguishable. Played through headphones, listeners sometimes reported a faint eighth note preceding each quarter note.

REFERENCES Desain, R. O. A (de)composable theory of rhythm perception. Music Perception, 1992, 9, 439-454. Gabrielsson, A. Timing in music performance and its relations to music experience. In J. A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative processes in music. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1988. Gjerdingen, R. O. Shape and motion in the microstructure of song. Music Perception, 1988, 6, 35-64. Gjerdingen, R. O. Meter as a mode of attending: A network simulation of attentional rhythmicity in music. Integral, 1989, 3, 67-92. Howell, P. Prediction of P-center location from the distribution of energy in the amplitude envelope: I. Perception & Psychophysics, 1988, 43, 90-93. Jones, M. R. Attentional rhythmicity in human perception. In J. R. Evans and M. Clynes (Eds.), Rhythm in psychological. linguistic. and musical processes. Springfield, III.: Charles Thomas Publishers, 1986. Morton, J., Marcus, S. M., & Frankish, C. Perceptual centers (P centers). Psychological Review, 1976, 83, 405-408. Vas, J., & Rasch, R. The perceptual onset of musical tones. Perception & Psychophysics, 1981, 29, 323-335.