the mathematics of the voice. As musicians, we d both been frustrated with groups inability to

Similar documents
WHAT IS BARBERSHOP. Life Changing Music By Denise Fly and Jane Schlinke

A comparison of the acoustic vowel spaces of speech and song*20

Oskaloosa Community School District. Music. Grade Level Benchmarks

Version 5: August Requires performance/aural assessment. S1C1-102 Adjusting and matching pitches. Requires performance/aural assessment

Welcome to Vibrationdata

Chapter Two: Long-Term Memory for Timbre

LEAD SECTIONAL. Expression Accurate sense of plan basic to complex Ability to craft a simple and successful plan, leave the interp for coaches

Mixture of styles in feedback reduces clarity of message to contestants Doesn t look joined up or coherent Doesn t present a professional image

In 2015 Ian Howell of the New England Conservatory introduced

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

Analysis of the effects of signal distance on spectrograms

The Art of Singing. Kyria Abrahams

The role of vocal tract resonances in singing and in playing wind instruments

Kent Academic Repository

Making music with voice. Distinguished lecture, CIRMMT Jan 2009, Copyright Johan Sundberg

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

DEVELOPING THE MALE HEAD VOICE. A Paper by. Shawn T. Eaton, D.M.A.

Marimba. When trying to decide what to do for my project, I came across the idea of

8/16/16. Clear Targets: Sound. Chapter 1: Elements. Sound: Pitch, Dynamics, and Tone Color

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Formant frequency tuning in singing

Is Your Piano Out of Tune?

Vocal-tract Influence in Trombone Performance

International Journal of Computer Architecture and Mobility (ISSN ) Volume 1-Issue 7, May 2013

Pitch-Synchronous Spectrogram: Principles and Applications

How to Use This Book and CD

Beethoven s Fifth Sine -phony: the science of harmony and discord

The Complete Vocal Workout for Guys

The Perception of Formant Tuning in Soprano Voices

BARBERSHOP BASICS (All you ever wanted to know about Barbershop, but were afraid to ask.)

IBEGIN MY FIRST ARTICLE AS Associate Editor of Journal of Singing for

Unit Outcome Assessment Standards 1.1 & 1.3

Tone for Clarinet Ensemble

Week 22, Unit 22: The French Sixth Chord and The Neapolitan Sixth Chord

Real-time magnetic resonance imaging investigation of resonance tuning in soprano singing

The Practice Room. Learn to Sight Sing. Level 3. Rhythmic Reading Sight Singing Two Part Reading. 60 Examples

EXPECTATIONS at the end of this unit. some children will not have made so much progress and will:

2014A Cappella Harmonv Academv Handout #2 Page 1. Sweet Adelines International Balance & Blend Joan Boutilier

Contest and Judging Manual

Music for the Hearing Care Professional Published on Sunday, 14 March :24

Harmonic Analysis of the Soprano Clarinet

INTERVALS Ted Greene

NORTHERN REGION MIDDLE SCHOOL FESTIVAL VOCAL REQUIREMENTS Read carefully, some items may have changed

HIGHLANDS CHOIR SEMESTER EXAM REVIEW. Whole Half Quarter Eighth Sixteenth. Whole Half Quarter Eighth Sixteenth

This document can be used for Men s and Women s Glee Clubs as well as beginning and intermediate choirs.] Table of Contents

Registration Reference Book

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. X-ray study of articulation and formant frequencies in two female singers

KVMEA Judges Handbook First Edition 2012

Evaluating trained singers tone quality and the effect of changing focus of attention on performance

Music Selection Prepared by Lorraine Barrows with input from past scripts by Carolyn Butler, Vicki Uhr, Marge Bailey, Joni Bescos.

Lecture 1: What we hear when we hear music

Section V: Technique Building V - 1

VOCAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE FOUR VOICE PARTS

Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas: Implications for intelligibility and ease of singing

Complete Vocal Technique in four pages

West Linn-Wilsonville School District Primary (Grades K-5) Music Curriculum. Curriculum Foundations

HORIZONTE INSTRUCTION AND TRAINING CENTER MUSIC APPRECIATION/CHORUS OPEN DISCLOSURE DOCUMENT

Lecture 7: Music

Sound Quality PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 32. Sound perception

Divisions on a Ground

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

Physics and Neurophysiology of Hearing

AUDITION PROCEDURES:

Pitch. There is perhaps no aspect of music more important than pitch. It is notoriously

Study Guide. Solutions to Selected Exercises. Foundations of Music and Musicianship with CD-ROM. 2nd Edition. David Damschroder

Personality Portrait. Joyce Ma and Fay Dearborn. November 2005

Office of Curriculum, Instruction, and Technology. A Cappella Choir. Grade 10, 11, or 12. Prerequisite: Concert Choir or Chorale.

Music Curriculum Kindergarten

3b- Practical acoustics for woodwinds: sound research and pitch measurements

Student Performance Q&A:

c l a s s r o o m a n d at t h e m a n y s k i l l s

CHAPTER CHAPTER CHAPTER CHAPTER CHAPTER CHAPTER CHAPTER CHAPTER CHAPTER 9...

SMCPS Course Syllabus

Partimenti Pedagogy at the European American Musical Alliance, Derek Remeš

Analysis of Caprice No. 42. Throughout George Rochberg s Caprice No. 42, I hear a kind of palindrome and inverse

The interaction between room and musical instruments studied by multi-channel auralization

(Adapted from Chicago NATS Chapter PVA Book Discussion by Chadley Ballantyne. Answers by Ken Bozeman)

FPFV-285/585 PRODUCTION SOUND Fall 2018 CRITICAL LISTENING Assignment

REPORT ON THE NOVEMBER 2009 EXAMINATIONS

The Practice Room. Learn to Sight Sing. Level 2. Rhythmic Reading Sight Singing Two Part Reading. 60 Examples

Outside - with George Garzone

Choir Scope and Sequence Grade 6-12

Habits. of a. How Successful Band Directors Leave a Musical and Personal Legacy. Scott Rush

AGO NATIONAL CONFERENCE IN BOSTON, JUNE 25, 2014 STRETCHING THE SKILLS OF CHURCH CHOIRS Lecture Outline

Doubletalk Detection

10 Lessons In Jazz Improvisation By Mike Steinel University of North Texas

The following General Music performance objectives are integrated throughout the entire course: MUSIC SKILLS

CONTENT AREA: MUSIC EDUCATION

The Physics Of Sound. Why do we hear what we hear? (Turn on your speakers)

2014 Music Style and Composition GA 3: Aural and written examination

Composer Style Attribution

Pitch Perception. Roger Shepard

An Integrated Music Chromaticism Model

Mind Formative Evaluation. Limelight. Joyce Ma and Karen Chang. February 2007

How We Sing: The Science Behind Our Musical Voice. Music has been an important part of culture throughout our history, and vocal

CLASSIFICATION OF THE METAPHORS ACCORDING TO THE DEGREE OF UNEXPECTEDNESS

Level of Difficulty: Beginning Prerequisites: None

The Black Book Series: The Lost Art of Magical Charisma (The Unreleased Volume: Beyond The 4 Ingredients)

Mixing in the Box A detailed look at some of the myths and legends surrounding Pro Tools' mix bus.

STEVE TADD WOODWIND REPAIRS (.co.uk)

Chetek-Weyerhaeuser High School

Transcription:

Bailey Hoar & Grace Lempres December 7, 2010 Math 005 Final Project Because we are both singers, we decided that we wanted our project to experiment with the mathematics of the voice. As musicians, we d both been frustrated with groups inability to tune notes when singing the same pitch on the same vowel (particularly the ooo vowel); we were both excited to learn the mathematical reasoning behind this phenomenon. Having learned about formants in class, we decided to combine our mathematical knowledge about singing with our musical knowledge. We began our project with this question: how does your vocal training affect the way you shape your vowels, and therefore your formants, when you sing? Given our backgrounds in singing in all-female groups, and our worries that analyzing both genders would be too complicated, we decided to test only female singers. We recorded twelve Dartmouth students, including ourselves, singing a 2-octave arpeggio; the higher singers started on A Flat 3, the lower singers on G Flat 3. Each girl sang the arpeggio on three vowels mee, mah, and moo. We then analyzed each recording using the spectrogram tool of praat; we chose to analyze the formants ourselves rather than use praat s Show Formants tool, which is built to analyze speech, not song. What we noticed as we recorded surprised us. Every girl who came to sing for us changed the shape of their vowel as they ascended in pitch, no matter what their training was. When we analyzed their highest note (A Flat 5 or G Flat 5), we discovered that their was little variation in perceived vowel sound. We noted that some girls could maintain a more distinct vowel in the higher range, but when we analyzed the formants of their highest note, there was virtually no difference between the and F2 values for mee, mah, and moo. We did

notice that girls with extensive classical training seemed to maintain their vowels better, but the formants were always the same regardless of vowel. We were puzzled by how intuitive this phenomenon was: every girl automatically changed her vowel as she ascended in pitch. When we asked our test subjects what they thought about when singing high notes, these are some of the things we heard: I create space in the back of my mouth and I open my mouth, so the vowel has to change, or it won t sound right I open everything more I open the back of my throat as if there is a very large egg in the back I aim for the best quality of sound. The vowels are not as important (relatively) when I sing high Put another way, when singers ascend in pitch, they are gradually lowering the jaw as they ascend, and/or by 'smiling' more as they ascend in pitch 1 they truly do open everything than can be opened. What our test subjects do intuitively, and what they are describing above, is an important phenomenon that occurs when singing. Acousticians at the University of New South Wales reported: Sopranos can sing at frequencies that are rather higher than the normal val- ues for the lowest resonance of their vocal tract, but failure to use this resonance would reduce both their vocal power and homogeneity in timbre. We have directly measured the resonance frequencies of the vocal tract of sopranos during singing, and find that, towards the top of their range, they consistently increase the frequency of the lowest resonance to match that of their singing. This significantly increases the loudness and the uniformity of tone, albeit at the expense of comprehensibility 2. Though these results were not unexpected, we were shocked at how naturally singers do this, and how widespread the tuning of formants to match resonant frequencies is. It seems to be, at least for women, a subconscious and obvious choice. 1 Sopranos: Resonance tuning and vowel changes. http://www.phys.unsw.edu.au/jw/soprane.html 2 Tuning of vocal tract resonance by sopranos. Joliveau, Smith, and Wolfe. Nature 2004. http://www.phys.unsw.edu.au/jw/reprints/sopranonat.pdf

Our observations and results for this part of the experiment were so homogenous that we decided to explore another aspect of formants. The one thing that most surprised us is how quickly singers will compromise vowel clarity and how unintentional this compromise is. It seems natural to all the singers we interviewed that they would change their vowel in order to get a better sound. We then wondered what would happen if you put emphasis on the vowel rather than the sound produced what would happen to the formants? What would happen to the sound? To explore this, we began to record singers singing not only as they usually would but also singing the same arpeggio with as distinct a vowel as possible. We don t have as much data for this part of our project, having already recorded several singers by the time we decided to record this additional data. The data we do have is very consistent; therefore, we think it is a good representation of what would have happened had we recorded all our subjects. The results were very different than the results we got for the neutral vowel. When we asked singers to maintain a mee all the way through the arpeggio, they were able to maintain the usual shape of the formants that you expect for a mee vowel. While the value of the squeaky mee, as we called it, was the same as for the neutral mee, the F2 value of the squeaky mee jumped up to where F3 was for the neutral mee. Where we used to see a strong band F2 for the neutral mee, almost nothing remained on the spectrogram for the squeaky mee. Equally important to note is that for the neutral mee and F2 were of equal strength, while the F2 of the squeaky mee was much stronger than the! Now is a good time to transition into the qualitative analysis of the squeaky mee. After our subjects recorded their second arpeggio, we asked them to describe how it felt and sounded to sing a distinct vowel so high in their range. Some responses:

That s hard ew! It hurt Feels tight Really awkward and difficult We also noticed that the sounds we recorded sounded much more forced; the singers we recorded expressed a similar distaste for the sounds they produced. Some of them didn t even understand why we would want them to make such a unpleasant sound in the first place; others couldn t even comprehend that we were asking them to do such an unintuitive thing. For singers, therefore, it is the natural choice to change to a neutral vowel rather than maintain the vowel sound because the sound is more forced and sounds worse. Why, however, does the sound sound worse? As we discussed, the strength of the very high F2 indicates that high harmonics are much more prevalent in the squeaky mee, leading to a much harsher timbre one that is less pleasant to listen to. Because the high notes our singers were recording are in such a sensitive part of the human hearing range, this harsh timbre seems particularly unpleasant. The harsh timbre would not only be less pleasant to listen to in a solo performance, but would also be much harder to blend with in a group. Singers already, as result of small variations in their vocal tract, have a hard time blending imagine a group of sopranos singing high notes in this fashion. The overwhelming high harmonics would no doubt make it impossible to produce a pleasant, blended sound or any blended sound, for that matter. Our spectrograms also displayed how physically difficult it was to produce this sound. All of our data is only on the mee vowel because it was impossible to maintain a moo that high in one s range. We noticed that, at the beginning of the highest note, there is a spike in the spectrogram as each singer attempted to maintain the vowel in their high register. We also noticed, after analyzing the formants, how great the variability in pitch was for the squeaky mee. Not only was it harder for the singer to maintain a consistent pitch when singing in this

way, but the variation in pitch across the subjects was astounding. Whereas with the neutral vowel singers seemed to agree on a frequency value, the squeaky mee generated a large variability in pitch. This variability again contributes to the unpleasant nature of the sound and again would make the note harder to tune in a group. What s more, when singing scales or mellismas, proper pitch so integral to the phrase would be incredibly hard to maintain. When a singer can t agree on the pitch of one note alone, tuning half and whole steps would be even more difficult. After analyzing the various problems with singing a squeaky mee, we compared our neutral mee to one other set of data: the and F2 values of mee, mah, and moo for the note an octave below (A Flat 4 or G Flat 4). As expected, we noticed a great difference between the and F2 values for mee and the and F2 values for mah and moo. What we didn t expect to see was how close our average and F2 values were for mah and moo. While these two vowels are certainly closer in shape than they are to a mee vowel, we still expected them to have disctinct and F2 values. We did notice a wider range of and F2 values for moo. As singers, we know how hard singers work to form their moo vowel, as well as how different ooo vowels can sound. Here, we discovered that training did play a part in the and F2 values: classically-trained singers had formants that were more dissimilar to the mah values, whereas girls with either no formal training or musical theater training had formant values that were very similar for moo and mah. For example, Anna P has no formal training, whereas Jen is a classical singer; Aislinn was trained in musical theater whereas Grace was trained classically. Finally, we had an answer to our question: training can affect formant vowels, but only in the low and middle parts of a singer s voice. Once singers hit their high register, however, they

naturally change their vowel in order to produce a better and more pleasant sound, one that is easier to sing, easier to keep in tune, and one that can resonate more easily. Maintaining the shape of the vowel in the high register results in unpleasantness that can be both noted on the spectrogram and in the data and heard aurally. No matter what your training, quality of sound is paramount, and every singer naturally does what she can to create the best sound possible.

Example Spectrograms Changing to a neutral mee: Singing a squeaky mee:

A Flat 5 Mee F2 Mee F3 Mee Mah F2 Mah F3 Mah Moo F2 Moo F3 Moo Danielle 830 1655 2500 845 1690 2540 846 1708 2570 Diana 832 1680 2540 830 1665 2490 832 1655 2515 Hannah 832 1680 2528 819 1667 2502 819 1665 2515 Anna F 819 1627 2436 832 1640 2475 819 1640 2490 Sara 822 1716 2558 835 1677 2532 822 1677 2545 Bailey 848 1690 2558 840 1665 2540 834 1650 2520 Grace 830 1650 2545 824 1676 2496 832 1665 2525 Aislinn 809 1638 2506 835 1664 2506 822 1651 2506 High Avg 827.75 1241.25 2521.375 832.5 1668 2510.125 828.25 1663.875 2523.25 G Flat 5 Mee F2 Mee F3 Mee Mah F2 Mah F3 Mah Moo F2 Moo F3 Moo Anna P 745 1470 2208 732 1483 2247 732 1496 2260 Jen 719 1457 2182 745 1522 2312 732 1483 2260 Annalea 705 1423 2176 706 1431 2182 719 1444 2156 Zana 719 1470 2221 706 1509 2324 758 1535 2286 Low Avg 722 1455 2196.75 722.25 1486.25 2266.25 735.25 1489.5 2240.5 A Flat 4 Mee F2 Mee Mah F2 Mah Moo F2 Moo Aislinn 400 2526 395 822 395 822 Sara 408 2519 421 822 408 835 Bailey 421 2532 422 826 420 860 Grace 416 2540 416 830 410 856 G Flat 4 Mee F2 Mee Mah F2 Mah Moo F2 Moo Anna P 356 2195 356 719 356 719 Jen 369 2195 369 732 369 783 Zana 356 2221 369 758 369 770 Annalea 369 2260 369 732 369 732 Squeaky Meee F2 Anna P 745 2234 Jen 783 2428 Zana 732 2299 Aislinn 822 2519 Bailey 835 2545

Math 5 Final Project Graphs NOTE: These graphs are meant to be compared as sets. Inconsistencies within the graphs are due to variations in pitch and are less relevant for this project. 1540 1520 1500 vs. F2 Moo: Altos 1480 1460 F2 Moo 1440 1420 710 720 730 740 750 760 vs. F2 Mah: Altos 1540 1520 1500 1480 1460 1440 1420 700 710 720 730 740 750 F2 Mah vs. F2 Mee: Altos 1480 1470 1460 1450 1440 F2 Mee 1430 1420 700 710 720 730 740 750

vs. F2 Mah 1700 1690 1680 1670 1660 1650 F2 Mah 1640 1630 815 820 825 830 835 840 845 850 vs. F2 Moo 1720 1710 1700 1690 1680 1670 1660 1650 1640 1630 815 820 825 830 835 840 845 850 F2 Moo vs. F2 Mee 1740 1720 1700 1680 1660 1640 1620 800 810 820 830 840 850 F2 Mee

Average Low Formant Values (A Flat 4) 2600 2400 2200 2000 1800 1600 1400 1200 1000 800 600 400 200 0 409404.60 408.6 2508 830840.6 Mee Mah Moo F2 1700 1600 1500 1400 1300 1200 1100 1000 900 800 700 600 500 400 300 200 100 0 828.3831.5 829 1660.5 1665.51667 Mee Mah Moo F2